Observation Compromises the Credibility of an Evaluation
Robert J Barth
Search for other papers by Robert J Barth in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Restricted access

Abstract

Impairment evaluations often occur within an adversarial context that involves a claimant or plaintiff vs a defense or benefits system; this adversarial context may precipitate proposals for observations of a clinical evaluation (eg, by an attorney or attorneys for one or both parties, a court reporter, a clinical expert, other consultant; audio or video also may be recorded). Evaluators, judges, and state workers’ compensation systems sometimes allow such observation, but a century of scientific research has reliably demonstrated that any observation changes an examinee's presentation in ways that are not themselves predictable. Such contamination leaves the evaluation results without a scientifically credible analysis, rendering observed evaluations futile exercises at best and sources of misinformation at worst. This article reviews the research in social psychology regarding “social facilitation and inhibition,” which has identified an extensive list of factors that are affected by observation (eg, complexity or novelty of the issue being evaluated, perceptions of the observer as an evaluator, stranger, or ally, number of observers, and other factors). No mechanism allows an evaluator to systematically account for the effects of all such variables for any given evaluation. When observation is mandatory, the evaluator should clearly document and communicate that a credible direct evaluation was rendered impossible, to the detriment of the referring party and the system as a whole.

  • 1.

    McCaffrey RJ, Fischer JM, Gold BA, and Lynch JK. Presence of third parties during neuropsychological evaluations: who is evaluating whom? The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 1996; (10) 4: 435449.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 2.

    Zelig, M. A forensic approach to fitness for duty evaluations. Workshop presented for the American Psychological Association, New Orleans, Louisiana, August 12, 2006.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 3.

    Zajonc RB. Social facilitation. Science. 1965; (149): 269275.

  • 4.

    Bond CF, and Titus LJ. Social facilitation: a meta-analysis of 241 studies. Psychological Bulletin. 1983; (94): 265292.

  • 5.

    McCaffrey RJ, Lynch JK, and Yantz CL. Third party observers: while all the fuss? J Forensic Neuropsychol. 2005; 4(2): 115.

  • 6.

    Lynch JK. Effect of a third-party observer on neuropsychological test performance following closed head injury. J Forensic Neuropsychol. 2005; 4 (2): 1725, 2005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 7.

    Seta JJ, Crisson JE, Seta CE, and Wang M.A. Task performance on perceptions of anxiety: averaging and summation in an evaluative setting. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989; (56): 387396.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 8.

    Seta JJ, Wang MA, Crisson JE, and Seta CE. Audience composition and felt anxiety: impact on averaging and summation. Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 1989; (10): 5772.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 9.

    Constantinou M, Ashendorf L, and McCaffrey RJ. When the third-party observer of a neuropsychological evaluation is an audio recorder. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2002;(16): 407412.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 10.

    Constantinou M, Ashendorf L, and McCaffrey RJ. Effects of a third-party observer during neuropsychological assessment: when the observer is a video camera. J Forensic Neuropsychol. 2005; 4(2): 3948.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 11.

    Putz VR. The effects of different modes of supervision of vigilance behavior. Bri J Psychol. 1975; 66: 157160.

  • 12.

    Geen RG. Effects of being observed on short and long term recall. J Exp Psychol. 1973; (100): 395398.

  • 13.

    Landers DM, Bauer RS, and Feltz DL. Social facilitation during the initial stage of motor learning: a re-examination of Marten's audience study. J of Motor Behav. 1978; (10): 325337.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 14.

    Cohen JL. Social facilitation: increased evaluation apprehension through permanency record. Motivation and Emotion. 1979; (3): 1933.

  • 15.

    Henchy T, and Glass DC. Evaluation apprehension and the social facilitation of dominant and subordinate responses. J Pers and Soc Psycho. 1968; (10): 446454.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 16.

    Yantz CL, and McCaffrey RJ. Effects of a supervisor's observation on memory test performance of the examinee: third-party observer effect confirmed. J Forensic Neuropsych. 2005; 4(2): 2738.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 17.

    Grindrod D, Paton CD, Knez WL, O'Brien BJ. Six minute walk distance is greater when performed in a group than alone. Br J Sports Med. 2006; Oct; 40(10):8767.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 18.

    Gavett BE, Lynch JK, and McCaffrey RJ. Third party observers: the effect size is greater than you might think. J Forensic Neuropsychol. 2005; 4(2): 4964.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 19.

    Packard RC. Posttraumatic headache: permanency and relationship to legal settlement. Headache. 1992; Nov; 32(10): 496500.

  • 20.

    Butler J, and Baumeister RF. The trouble with friendly faces: skilled performance with a supportive audience. J Pers and Soc Psychol. 1998; (75): 12131230.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • 21.

    McCaffrey RJ. Some final thoughts and comments regarding the issues of third party observers. J Forensic Neuropsychol. 2005; 4(2): 8391.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 943 943 196
Full Text Views 32 32 0
PDF Downloads 0 0 0
Save